Refine
Year of publication
Document Type
- Doctoral Thesis (65)
- Article (57)
- Report (1)
Has Fulltext
- yes (123)
Is part of the Bibliography
- no (123)
Keywords
- - (50)
- Angst (7)
- depression (7)
- emotion (6)
- Depression (4)
- Emotion (4)
- Emotionsregulation (4)
- Entscheidung (4)
- Furcht (4)
- clinical psychology (4)
- social cognition (4)
- Angstsensitivität (3)
- Ereigniskorreliertes Potenzial (3)
- Kognition (3)
- Lebensqualität (3)
- Motivation (3)
- Physiologische Psychologie (3)
- Trauma (3)
- anxiety sensitivity (3)
- attention (3)
- distress (3)
- emotion recognition (3)
- fear (3)
- psychotherapy (3)
- quality of life (3)
- risk perception (3)
- Affekt (2)
- Alter (2)
- Angststörung (2)
- Borderline-Persönlichkeitsstörung (2)
- Commitment (2)
- Drittmittelanalyse (2)
- Emotionen (2)
- Erleben (2)
- Forschung (2)
- Gefühl (2)
- HRV (2)
- Handlungstheorie (2)
- Impulsivität (2)
- Intelligenz (2)
- Inter-Gruppenbeziehung (2)
- Interozeption (2)
- Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie (2)
- Kulturkontakt (2)
- Messung (2)
- Panic disorder (2)
- Panikstörung (2)
- Posttraumatische Belastungsstörung (2)
- Psychopathologie (2)
- Publikationsanalyse (2)
- Risikowahrnehmung (2)
- Schizophrenie (2)
- Stress (2)
- Versorgungszufriedenheit (2)
- Ziel (2)
- anxiety (2)
- borderline personality disorder (2)
- cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy (2)
- cognitive control (2)
- disaster (2)
- emotion regulation (2)
- empathy (2)
- evaluation (2)
- fMRI (2)
- face perception (2)
- health-related quality of life (2)
- heart rate variability (2)
- heartbeat detection (2)
- interoception (2)
- interoceptive threat (2)
- kortikale Verarbeitung (2)
- mental health (2)
- posttraumatic stress disorder (2)
- psychophysiology (2)
- public health (2)
- publication analysis (2)
- questionnaire development (2)
- reappraisal (2)
- research (2)
- research funding analysis (2)
- suppression (2)
- vagal tone (2)
- vagus nerve (2)
- (Simplified) Conjoint-Expected-Risk Model (1)
- ADHD (1)
- ADHS (1)
- ALS (1)
- Action theory (1)
- Adaptation (1)
- Agoraphobia (1)
- Agoraphobie (1)
- Alkohol (1)
- Alkoholabhängigkeit (1)
- Alkoholismus (1)
- Altern (1)
- Altersangst (1)
- Alterschirurgie (1)
- Alterspsychologie (1)
- Ambulantes Assessment (1)
- Amyotrophe Lateralsklerose (1)
- Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (1)
- Angststörungen (1)
- Anstrengung (1)
- Anxiety Disorders (1)
- Anxiety sensitivity (1)
- Arbeitsleistung (1)
- Arbeitsplatzunsicherheit (1)
- Arbeitspsychologie (1)
- Arbeitswissenschaft (1)
- Assoziation (1)
- Atemnot , Interozeption , Paniksyndrom , Physiologische Psychologie (1)
- Atmung (1)
- Aufmerksamkeit (1)
- Aufmerksamkeits-Defizit-Syndrom (1)
- Aufmerksamkeitsbias (1)
- Autonome Erregung (1)
- BED (1)
- Bedrohung (1)
- Belohnung (1)
- Bevölkerung (1)
- Bewusstseinslage (1)
- Bezugsnorm (1)
- Bias (1)
- Big Five (1)
- Binaural beats (1)
- Bindungstyp (1)
- Biowissenschaften (1)
- Borderline Personality Disorder (1)
- Brain Stimulation (1)
- Brain stimulation (1)
- Bulimia Nervosa (1)
- Bulimie (1)
- CBASP (1)
- COVID-19 (1)
- COVID-19 pandemic (1)
- Children Born of War (1)
- Cognition (1)
- Cognitive-behavioral therapy (1)
- Common Ingroup Identity (1)
- Comorbidity (1)
- Complex Problem Solving (1)
- Computersimulation (1)
- Cortisol (1)
- Crisis communication (1)
- Crisis situation (1)
- DISMA (1)
- Decision making (1)
- Deferral (1)
- Delay Aversion (1)
- Deprivation (1)
- Deutsche Gesellschaft für Psychologie (1)
- Diagnosen (1)
- Dialektisch behaviorale Therapie (1)
- Dialektisch-Behaviorale Therapie (1)
- Dilemma (1)
- Distanz (1)
- Diversität (1)
- Donor return behavior (1)
- Dopamin (1)
- Dual-task (1)
- Duchenne (1)
- ECG (1)
- EEG (1)
- EKP (1)
- EMDR (1)
- ERP (1)
- ERPs (1)
- Eignungsdiagnostik (1)
- Einsatzkräfte (1)
- Ekel (1)
- Electronic Shopping (1)
- Embodiment (1)
- Emetophobie (1)
- Emotion Regulation (1)
- Emotionales Verhalten (1)
- Emotionsausdruck (1)
- Emotionsinduktion (1)
- Entscheidungsforschung (1)
- Entwicklungskrise (1)
- Entwicklungspsychologie (1)
- Epidemiologie (1)
- Epidemiology (1)
- Episodisches Gedächtnis (1)
- Ergebnisqualität (1)
- Ergonomie (1)
- Erheiterung (1)
- Erstickungsangst (1)
- Erwartung (1)
- Escalation of Commitment (1)
- Essensbilder (1)
- Evaluation (1)
- Exposition (1)
- Exposure (1)
- Expressed Emotion (1)
- Extinktion (1)
- Eye Tracking (1)
- FACS (1)
- Facelift (1)
- False-Belief-Test (1)
- Fehleinschätzung (1)
- Feuerwehr (1)
- First-time donors (1)
- Fokus Gruppen (1)
- Food cues (1)
- Freundschaft (1)
- Furchtextinktion (1)
- Furchtkonditionierung (1)
- Gedächtnis (1)
- Gefangene (1)
- Gefangener (1)
- Gefängnis (1)
- Gefühlspsychologie (1)
- Gefühlsreaktion (1)
- German Psychological Society (1)
- Gerontopsychologie (1)
- Geschlechtergerechtigkeit (1)
- Geschwindigkeit (1)
- Gesundheitspsychologie (1)
- Grundschule (1)
- Gruppe (1)
- Guppenbindung (1)
- HIV (1)
- Handlung (1)
- Handlungs-Ergebnis-Erwartung (1)
- Handlungsmodell (1)
- Health behaviour (theories) (1)
- Heimtest (1)
- Hippocampal network integrity (1)
- Hirnstimulation (1)
- Hyperventilation (1)
- IQ (1)
- Imagination (1)
- Impulsivity (1)
- Inanspruchnahme (1)
- Informationsverarbeitung (1)
- Informationsverzerrung (1)
- Ingroup-Bias (1)
- Initially deferred donors (1)
- Innovation (1)
- Institution (1)
- Instrumentenentwicklung (1)
- Intelligence (1)
- Intensität (1)
- Intention (1)
- Intention forming (1)
- Intentionsbildung (1)
- Intergruppeneinstellung (1)
- Intergruppenkontakt (1)
- Interkulturelle Kompetenz (1)
- International (1)
- Interoception (1)
- Interview (1)
- Job Performance (1)
- Katastrophen (1)
- Kausalität (1)
- Kernspintomografie (1)
- Klinische Psychologie (1)
- Knowledge (1)
- Kognitive Entwicklung (1)
- Kognitive Psychologie (1)
- Kombi-Therapie (1)
- Komplexes Problemlösen (1)
- Konditionierung (1)
- Kontrolle (1)
- Kooperation (1)
- Krise der Lebensmitte (1)
- Krisensituationen (1)
- Kunde (1)
- Kur (1)
- Latenz (1)
- Lebensereignis (1)
- Leistungsangst (1)
- Lernen (1)
- Life Event (1)
- MEPS (1)
- MIMIC model (1)
- MND (1)
- MRI (1)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging (1)
- Memory function (1)
- Mentale Beanspruchung (1)
- Messtheorie (1)
- Messung / Psychologie (1)
- Mimik (1)
- Mindset (1)
- Misserfolg (1)
- Mitarbeiterbindung (1)
- Mood (1)
- Moral (1)
- Motoneuronerkrankungen (1)
- Motor Neuron Diseases (1)
- Myatrophische Lateralsklerose (1)
- Nahrung (1)
- Nahrungsreize (1)
- Narzissmus (1)
- Neoinstitutionalismus (1)
- Neurobiologie (1)
- Neurobiology (1)
- Neuronale Netzwerke (1)
- Neuropsychologie (1)
- Neuropsychology (1)
- Neurose (1)
- Neurotizismus (1)
- Notfall (1)
- Notfalleinsatz (1)
- Notfallerfahrung (1)
- Notfallversorgung (1)
- Notfallvorsorge (1)
- Notfallwissen (1)
- Objekterkennung (1)
- P300 (1)
- PRP (1)
- Paarvergleich (1)
- Panic attack (1)
- Panik (1)
- Panikattacke (1)
- Paniksyndrom (1)
- Persistenz (1)
- Personalauswahl (1)
- Personalentwicklung (1)
- Persönlichkeitsstruktur (1)
- Phobie (1)
- Physiologische Stressreaktion (1)
- Planspiel (1)
- Plastische Chirurgie (1)
- Posttraumatisches Stresssyndrom (1)
- Primacy-Effekt (1)
- Problemlösen (1)
- Problemlöseszenario (1)
- Protective Action Decision Model (1)
- Prozessqualität (1)
- Präferenz (1)
- Prävention (1)
- Prüfungsangst (1)
- Prüfungsangstfragebogen (1)
- Prüfungsangstmodell (1)
- Prüfungsangstprädiktoren (1)
- Psychische Belastung (1)
- Psychische Störung (1)
- Psychischer Stress (1)
- Psychologische Diagnostik (1)
- Psychometrie (1)
- Psychopathology (1)
- Psychophysiologie (1)
- Psychotherapeutische Hochschulambulanz (1)
- Qualitätsmanagement (1)
- Qualitätssicherung (1)
- Randomized controlled trial (1)
- Rauchen (1)
- Reaktionszeit (1)
- Reappraisal (1)
- Rechtsextremismus (1)
- Reifung (1)
- Reihenfolge (1)
- Rekonsolidierung (1)
- Rubikon (1)
- Rumination (1)
- SBMA (1)
- Satisfaction with Care (1)
- Scham (1)
- Schnelltest (1)
- Schrecken (1)
- Schreckreaktion (1)
- Schreckreaktionen (1)
- Schreckreflex (1)
- Schuld (1)
- Schwierigkeit (1)
- Schönheitsoperation (1)
- Selbstbewertung (1)
- Selbstdiagnose (1)
- Selbstmanagement-Therapie (1)
- Selbsttest (1)
- Selbstwert (1)
- Selbstwirksamkeit (1)
- Sequenzlernen (1)
- Skalierung (1)
- Social problem-solving (1)
- Soziale Phobie (1)
- Soziale Unterstützung (1)
- Spezifische Phobie (1)
- Strukturqualität (1)
- Symptom (1)
- Tagungsgestaltung (1)
- Teams (1)
- Technikaffinität (1)
- Telemedizin (1)
- Testkonstruktion (1)
- Theorien des Gesundheitsverhaltens (1)
- Theory of Mind (1)
- Therapie (1)
- Transition (1)
- Transition Competence (1)
- Transitionskompetenz (1)
- Verhaltenstest (1)
- Verlaufsstudie (1)
- Vernetzung (1)
- Vernetzung ambulant - stationär (1)
- Vertrauen (1)
- Vertreibung (1)
- Verzögerungsaversion (1)
- Videodefiziteffekt (1)
- Visuelle Aufmerksamkeit (1)
- Volition (1)
- Volitional Strength (1)
- Volitionsstärke (1)
- Vorurteil (1)
- Vorurteile (1)
- Wahrnehmung (1)
- Warnung (1)
- Wiedererkennen (1)
- Wiedererkennungsgedächtnis (1)
- Willensfreiheit (1)
- Wissen (1)
- Wissenschaftspsychologie (1)
- Wohlbefinden (1)
- World War II (1)
- Wunschmedizin (1)
- Zeitmessung (1)
- Zielbindung (1)
- Zielorientierung (1)
- Zwangsstörung (1)
- Zweiter Weltkrieg (1)
- adulthood (1)
- adults (1)
- affect heuristic (1)
- affective neuroscience (1)
- affektive Dysregulation (1)
- age differences (1)
- agoraphobia (1)
- alcohol (1)
- alcohol dependency (1)
- alexithymia (1)
- amygdala (1)
- anxiety disorders (1)
- arousal (1)
- assessment/diagnosis (1)
- attentional bias (1)
- attitude measurement (1)
- autonomic arousal (1)
- behavioral avoidance test (1)
- behaviour intention (1)
- biases (1)
- blood donation (1)
- brain signal complexity (BSC) (1)
- burden (1)
- caffeine (1)
- cancer (1)
- case illustration (1)
- causal models (1)
- causal reasoning (1)
- cerebral palsy (1)
- character strengths (1)
- childhood maltreatment (1)
- children (1)
- chronic conditions (1)
- chronic depression (1)
- chronic disease (1)
- chronic kidney disease (1)
- chronische Erkrankungen (1)
- clinical controll group (1)
- cognition(s) (1)
- cognitive behavioral therapy (1)
- cognitive reappraisal (1)
- common cause hypothesis (1)
- common ingroup identity (1)
- comorbidity (1)
- conference organization (1)
- conflict (1)
- consciousness (1)
- creative thinking (1)
- creative/fluent verbal association (1)
- cross-sectional (1)
- crossover Design (1)
- decision-making (1)
- defensive Reaktivität (1)
- defensive flexibility (1)
- defensive reactivity (1)
- deprivation (1)
- deterrents (1)
- diabetes (1)
- diagnostic reasoning (1)
- diagnostisches Schließen (1)
- dialectical behavioral therapie (1)
- dialysis (1)
- disgust (1)
- displacement (1)
- distance (1)
- divergent thinking (DT) (1)
- dominance structure (1)
- dual task (1)
- eGFR (1)
- electroconvulsive therapy (1)
- elementary school (1)
- embodied cognition (1)
- emergency (1)
- emergency alert (1)
- emergency experience (1)
- emergency knowledge (1)
- emotion perception (1)
- emotional memory (1)
- emotional reactivity (1)
- emotionales Gedächtnis (1)
- empathetic concern (1)
- enteric hard capsule (1)
- estrogen (1)
- evaluation anxiety (1)
- event-related potential (1)
- event-related potentials (1)
- expectancy (1)
- experimentell (1)
- exposure therapy (1)
- eye-tracking (1)
- fMRT (1)
- face memory (1)
- facial expression (1)
- familienfreundliche Wissenschaft (1)
- family-friendly science (1)
- fear extinction (1)
- firefighter (1)
- fluid (gf) and crystallized intelligence (gc) (1)
- fluid intelligence (1)
- focus groups (1)
- follow-up treatment (1)
- free will (1)
- freedom (1)
- funktional (1)
- funktionelle MRT (1)
- gamma (1)
- gender differences (1)
- gender equality (1)
- general mental ability (1)
- goal (1)
- grenzüberschreitende Gesundheitsversorgung (1)
- group commitment (1)
- group therapy (1)
- harm avoidance (1)
- hazard type (1)
- health service research (1)
- hidden populations (1)
- high-frequency heart rate variability (1)
- home test (1)
- human–machine interaction (1)
- hydroxypropyl methyl cellulose (1)
- hydroxypropyl methyl cellulose acetate succinate (1)
- idealism (1)
- identity (1)
- immediate and delayed memory (1)
- impairments (1)
- incremental validity (1)
- individual differences (1)
- infection prevention (1)
- information distortion (1)
- information processing (1)
- ingroup bias (1)
- inhibition (1)
- initial panic attack (1)
- initiale Panikattacke (1)
- instrumentality of harm (1)
- intensified treatment (1)
- intercultural competence (1)
- intergroup attitudes (1)
- intergroup contact (1)
- interkulturelle Unterschiede (1)
- internet interventions (1)
- interozeptive Bedrohung (1)
- interpersonal skills (1)
- interprofessional attitude (1)
- interprofessional education (1)
- job insecurity (1)
- job performance (1)
- judgment (1)
- kausale Modelle (1)
- kausales Schlussfolgern (1)
- kognitive Entwicklungspsychologie (1)
- latency of affect (1)
- latency of object recognition (1)
- latent class analysis (1)
- latent class models (1)
- life-span development (1)
- locus coeruleus (1)
- longitudinal design (1)
- longitudinal research (1)
- maintenance (1)
- manual assembly (1)
- maximum performance (1)
- measurement/psychometrics (1)
- memory (1)
- menstrual cycle (1)
- mental chronometrics (1)
- mental disorder (1)
- mental illness (1)
- mental workload (1)
- mentale Rotation (1)
- mentale Transformation (1)
- metacognitive beliefs (1)
- metacognitive therapy (1)
- mindreading (1)
- moral decision making (1)
- moral dilemmas (1)
- moral judgment (1)
- moral reasoning (1)
- motivationaler Zustand (1)
- motives (1)
- multi-scale entropy (MSE) (1)
- multitasking (1)
- naturalistisch (1)
- near-hand space (1)
- network analysis (1)
- norepinephrine (1)
- observational study (1)
- oncology (1)
- online experiment (1)
- open data (1)
- open materials (1)
- oral contraceptives (1)
- outpatient psychotherapy clinic (1)
- pain (1)
- panic disorder (1)
- parental cancer (1)
- patient-reported outcome (1)
- patient‐reported outcome measures (PROM) (1)
- peripersonal space (1)
- peritraumatic reaction (1)
- peritraumatische Reaktionen (1)
- persistent depressive disorder (1)
- personal force (1)
- personality structure (1)
- perspective taking (1)
- phobia/phobic disorders (1)
- physical health (1)
- positive and negative feedback (1)
- positives und negatives Feedback (1)
- posttraumatic growth (1)
- posttraumatic stress (1)
- posttraumatischer Stress (1)
- posttraumatisches Wachstum (1)
- pre-hospital emergency care (1)
- prejudice (1)
- preregistered (1)
- prevalence (1)
- prevention (1)
- primacy effect (1)
- prison (1)
- prisoner (1)
- process dissociation procedure (1)
- processes and treatment outcomes (1)
- progesterone (1)
- psychiatric rehabilitation (1)
- psychische Gesundheit (1)
- psychological refractory period (PRP) (1)
- psychology of science (1)
- psychopathology (1)
- public warning systems (1)
- quality assurance (1)
- quality management (1)
- quality of structures (1)
- randomized controlled trial (1)
- rapid serial visual presentation (1)
- rapid test (1)
- recognition memory (1)
- reconsolidation (1)
- rejection sensitivity (1)
- representative sample (1)
- reproductive health (1)
- research participation (1)
- response bias (1)
- right-wing extremism (1)
- risk communication (1)
- rotating spot method (1)
- routine outcome monitoring and feedback (1)
- rubicon model (1)
- saliva (1)
- salivary alpha-amylase (1)
- satisfaction with care (1)
- schizophrenia (1)
- script-driven imagery (1)
- self management therapy (1)
- self-diagnosis (1)
- self-perception (1)
- self-testing (1)
- semantic vs. affective primacy (1)
- sensory processing (1)
- sequence learning (1)
- sex differences (1)
- sexual health (1)
- simultaneity judgments (1)
- smile expressions (1)
- smoking (1)
- social anxiety disorder (1)
- social interaction (1)
- social skills (1)
- soziale Fähigkeiten (1)
- spatial cognition (1)
- spatial orientation (1)
- spatial representation (1)
- spatial updating (1)
- specific phobia (1)
- speeded reaction time task (1)
- spider phobia (1)
- spontaneous reminding (1)
- spontanes Erinnern (1)
- stable isotope (1)
- startle eyeblink (1)
- startle responses (1)
- stigma (1)
- stochastic feedback (1)
- stochastisches Feedbacksignal (1)
- stress (1)
- strukturelle MRT (1)
- study protocol (1)
- suffocation fear (1)
- survivors (1)
- tVNS (1)
- task complexity (1)
- teenagers (1)
- telemedicine (1)
- temporal order judgments (1)
- test anxiety (1)
- test anxiety inventory (1)
- test anxiety model (1)
- test anxiety predictors (1)
- thematic analysis (1)
- therapeutic process (1)
- therapeutic strategies (1)
- therapeutische Strategien (1)
- trait anxiety (1)
- trajectories (1)
- transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (1)
- trauma (1)
- underdetermination (1)
- utilization (1)
- velocity (1)
- verbal creativity (1)
- vision (1)
- visual processing (1)
- volition (1)
- vulnerable populations (1)
- wahrgenommene Vulnerabilität (1)
- wahrgenommener Schweregrad (1)
- wahrgenommenes Risiko (1)
- warning message (1)
- weather (1)
- well-being (1)
- Ärger (1)
- Ästhetik (1)
- Überraschung (1)
Institute
- Institut für Psychologie (123) (remove)
Publisher
- Frontiers Media S.A. (27)
- MDPI (9)
- Frontiers (5)
- Hogrefe Verlag (4)
- S. Karger AG (4)
- GMS (1)
- Hogrefe Publishing (1)
- John Wiley & Sons, Ltd (1)
- Public Library of Science (PLoS) (1)
- SAGE Publications (1)
A long-standing controversy in emotion research concerns the question whether stimuli must be conceptually interpreted, or semantically categorized, to evoke emotional reactions. According to the semantic primacy hypothesis, the answer to this question is positive; whereas according to the affective primacy hypothesis, it is negative: Emotions can also be, and perhaps often are, elicited by preconceptual stimulus representations, such as particular shapes or color patterns.
In the present dissertation project, the semantic primacy hypothesis was tested in eight experiments using different latency judgment paradigms in which the perceptual latencies of object recognition and affect onset were measured and compared. The chronometric measurement methods comprised temporal judgments (temporal order judgments and simultaneity judgments: Publication A, Experiments 1–4; the rotating spot / rotating clock hand method: Publication B, Experiments 1–2) and speeded reaction time measurements (Publication C, Experiments 1–2). To elicit affective responses, pictures of pleasant (e.g., cats, children) and unpleasant objects (e.g., spiders, moldy food) from everyday life were presented.
According to the semantic primacy hypothesis, object recognition is a necessary partial cause of affect. This implies the following three predictions that were tested in the studies: (1) Because causes must precede their effects, the time of the onset of object recognition must precede the time of the onset of affect. (2) The longer it takes a person to recognize an object, the longer it should also take them, other factors constant, to experience affect; therefore, the latencies of the two mental events should be positively correlated across individuals. (3) An experimental manipulation that delays the onset of object recognition (in this case a moderate blurring of the pictures) should also delay the onset of affect, and the effect of the manipulation on affect latency should be mediated by the delay in object recognition.
In agreement with Prediction 1, regardless of the chronometric method used, the latency of object recognition consistently proved to be shorter than the latency of affect onset. According to the meta-analytically integrated latency differences estimated in the temporal judgment experiments, affect followed object recognition with a delay of 117 ms. This result was obtained for both pleasant and unpleasant stimuli and was independent of task order. Supporting Prediction 2, the latencies for object recognition and affect onset were positively correlated across participants (meta-analytic r = .50). Supporting Prediction 3, delaying object recognition by blurring the affective pictures was found to also delay the onset of affect and the effect of blurring on the latency of affect was found to be partly mediated by delayed object recognition.
Two additional predictions tested and confirmed in Experiment C2 were: (4) False-coloring the affective pictures delays the onset of affect but not object recognition, and this effect is mediated by reduced affect intensity. (5) Judgments of the valence of the stimuli (i.e., whether the imaged object is pleasant or unpleasant) take more time than reports of object recognition, but less time than affect onset reports, for which valence judgments have often been used as a substitute in previous studies.
Taken together, the results of the eight experiments provided consistent support for semantic primacy in the generation of pleasant and unpleasant feelings evoked by affective pictures: Object recognition can be considered a necessary partial cause of affect in the reported experiments. The results are compared to previous findings, possible reasons for deviant response patterns found in a small minority of the participants are considered, and several implications of the findings for emotion research are derived. Possible adaptations of the chronometric approach to investigate other questions of emotion research are suggested. Finally, limitations of the dissertation project are pointed out and possible ways to address these in future research are proposed.
Die zunehmende Digitalisierung und Technologisierung sorgt branchenübergreifend für eine Verlagerung der subjektiv erlebten Beanspruchung von physischer hin zu mentaler Beanspruchung. Um Arbeitsabläufe hinsichtlich auftretender Schwankungen mentaler Beanspruchung optimierbar zu machen, muss diese in Echtzeit am Arbeitsplatz erfassbar sein. Die Verwendung physiologischer Messinstrumente wie Elektrokardiogramm, Eye Tracking und Hautleitfähigkeit bieten dabei eine Möglichkeit der objektiven Quantifizierung der auftretenden Schwankungen. Im Rahmen verschiedener Feld- und Laborstudien konnte gezeigt werden, dass, in Abhängigkeit der Analyseeinheit (gesamter Prozess oder einzelne beanspruchungsinduzierende Events), unterschiedliche physiologische Parameter in der Lage sind Veränderungen der mentalen Beanspruchung nachzuweisen. Insgesamt erwiesen sich dabei die Herzfrequenz sowie die Ausdehnung der Pupille als sensitivste Indikatoren. Für eine live Erfassung im Arbeitsprozess bedarf es zukünftig, neben der Weiterentwicklung von tragbarer Messmethodik (Wearables), eine Entwicklung neuer Algorithmen zur Kombination verschiedener Parameter zu einem allgemeinen Indikator für mentale Beanspruchung, sowie der Bearbeitung einiger theoretischer Probleme, wie unter anderem der Definition von Grenzwerten der mentalen Beanspruchung. Um abseits bestehender Probleme Veränderungen der Beanspruchung am konkreten Arbeitsplatz einschätzbar zu machen, wurde eine alternative Auswertungsstrategie basierend auf kurzfristigen Peaks und längerfristigen Plateaus vorgeschlagen.
Gefahrenlagen, wie schwere Unwetter, Terroranschläge oder die COVID-19-Pandemie, stellen aktuell und zukünftig eine Bedrohung unserer Gesellschaft dar. Im Fall dieser und weiterer Gefahren können Warnungen helfen, Schäden zu verhindern und Menschenleben zu retten, indem sie die Empfänger*innen informieren und Schutzmaßnahmen vermitteln. Das Protective Action Decision Model (PADM) (Lindell & Perry, 2012) bietet einen theoretischen Rahmen, der Verarbeitungsprozesse von Warnungen und die Entstehung von Schutzverhalten abbildet. Neben zahlreichen weiteren Elementen beinhaltet das PADM die Wahrnehmung von Risiko als zentralen Faktor. Im Sinne des Modells sowie bereits existierender Literatur wird Risikowahrnehmung jedoch häufig ausschließlich kognitiv abgebildet. Zudem untersuchen Studien vorwiegend einzelne Gefahrenlagentypen oder singuläre Ereignisse.
Die vorliegende Arbeit bildet mit drei Beobachtungsstudien sowie einer experimentellen Studie Verarbeitungsprozesse von Warnungen vor verschiedenen Gefahrenlagen ab. Untersucht wurde der Einfluss der Warnungen auf kognitive und affektive Facetten der Risikowahrnehmung und ihre Rolle bei der Suche nach Informationen sowie der Intention, Schutzverhalten auszuführen. Über Online-Befragungen erhielten die Teilnehmenden Warnungen zu verschiedenen Gefahrenlagen (schweres Unwetter, Großbrand, extreme Gewalttat, Ausfall der Notrufnummer, Fund einer Weltkriegsbombe, COVID-19-Pandemie, Gewitter), die Informationen zur Gefahr sowie Handlungsempfehlungen enthielten. Befragt wurden sie unter anderem hinsichtlich ihrer Risikowahrnehmung vor und nach Warnerhalt sowie ihrer Intention, die angegebenen Schutzmaßnahmen zu befolgen oder sich Informationen zu suchen. Zudem wurden Eigenschaften der Warnungsempfänger*innen erhoben.
Die Ergebnisse stärken die Rolle affektiver Risikowahrnehmung für die Verarbeitung
von Warnungen sowie die Entstehung von Schutzverhalten und Informationssuche. Dies gilt
jedoch nicht für alle Gefahrenlagen gleichermaßen, sodass der Einfluss von Eigenschaften der Gefahr, wie Häufigkeit oder Schweregrad, deutlich wird. Bezüglich der Eigenschaften der Empfänger*innen ergab sich ebenfalls kein einheitliches Bild. Basierend auf den Ergebnissen wird eine Erweiterung des PADM um ein Modellelement der affektiven Risikowahrnehmung vorgeschlagen.
Fortführende Forschung zu Warnungen sollte eine multifacettierte Sichtweise von Risikowahrnehmung anstreben. Darüber hinaus sollten Gefahrenlagen vergleichend untersucht und ihre Eigenschaften sowie Eigenschaften der Warnungen systematisch variiert werden.
Introduction: To maintain a sufficient donor pool, deferred first-time donors (FTD) should be motivated to return for blood donation. This pilot study investigates how deferral affects momentary mood, satisfaction with the donation process, and subsequent return behavior to examine their potential for motivating (deferred) FTD. Methods: All of the subjects (n = 96) completed a first questionnaire (A1) before pre-donation assessment. Deferred FTD (n = 22) were asked to complete a second questionnaire (A2) immediately after deferral, while non-deferred FTD (n = 74) filled in the second questionnaire (A3) after blood donation. The impact of deferral, momentary mood, and satisfaction with the donation process on return behavior within 12 months was tested by calculating two path analyses, controlling for sex and age. Results: Mood (p < 0.001) and satisfaction with social aspects of the donation process (p = 0.01) were decreased after deferral. Deferred FTD were less likely than non-deferred FTD to return to the blood donation center within 12 months (60.8 vs. 36.4%; p = 0.043). However, path analyses revealed that deferral effects on mood and satisfaction were not connected to return behavior. Instead, age had a significant influence on return behavior (p < 0.05) such that, overall, non-returning FTD were older than returning FTD, regardless of their deferral status. Conclusion: Our findings suggest that mood and satisfaction with the donation process are directly affected by deferral but not clearly responsible for low return rates. It seems promising to embed these variables in established health behavior models in further studies to increase the return rates of deferred FTD.
Abstract
Background
The need to optimize exposure treatments for anxiety disorders may be addressed by temporally intensified exposure sessions. Effects on symptom reduction and public health benefits should be examined across different anxiety disorders with comorbid conditions.
Methods
This multicenter randomized controlled trial compared two variants of prediction error‐based exposure therapy (PeEx) in various anxiety disorders (both 12 sessions + 2 booster sessions, 100 min/session): temporally intensified exposure (PeEx‐I) with exposure sessions condensed to 2 weeks (n = 358) and standard nonintensified exposure (PeEx‐S) with weekly exposure sessions (n = 368). Primary outcomes were anxiety symptoms (pre, post, and 6‐months follow‐up). Secondary outcomes were global severity (across sessions), quality of life, disability days, and comorbid depression.
Results
Both treatments resulted in substantial improvements at post (PeEx‐I: dwithin = 1.50, PeEx‐S: dwithin = 1.78) and follow‐up (PeEx‐I: dwithin = 2.34; PeEx‐S: dwithin = 2.03). Both groups showed formally equivalent symptom reduction at post and follow‐up. However, time until response during treatment was 32% shorter in PeEx‐I (median = 68 days) than PeEx‐S (108 days; TRPeEx‐I = 0.68). Interestingly, drop‐out rates were lower during intensified exposure. PeEx‐I was also superior in reducing disability days and improving quality of life at follow‐up without increasing relapse.
Conclusions
Both treatment variants focusing on the transdiagnostic exposure‐based violation of threat beliefs were effective in reducing symptom severity and disability in severe anxiety disorders. Temporally intensified exposure resulted in faster treatment response with substantial public health benefits and lower drop‐out during the exposure phase, without higher relapse. Clinicians can expect better or at least comparable outcomes when delivering exposure in a temporally intensified manner.
Abstract
Lately, the use of patient‐reported outcome measures (PROM) to adapt and improve ongoing psychotherapeutic treatments has become more widespread. Their main purpose is to support data‐informed, collaborative treatment decisions which include the patient's point of view on their progress. In case of nonresponse or deterioration, these systems are able to warn clinicians and guide the process “back on track” in treatment. In this case illustration, the Greifswald Psychotherapy Navigator System (GPNS) detected the deterioration of 19‐year‐old Sarah during the first eight sessions of cognitive‐behavioral therapy for social anxiety and depression. Here, the GPNS helped the therapist gain insight as to how Sarah's social anxiety affected their treatment and adjust her strategy accordingly. Using the symptom curves and progress scales of the GPNS, the therapist was able to then address her patient's struggles in detail during their sessions and with her supervisor. After adapting her therapeutic approach, the patient's deterioration could be averted while simultaneously strengthening their communication in the process. Clinical implications and the benefits of using PROM systems for evidence‐based personalization of psychotherapy are presented.
Body dissatisfaction is pervasive among young women in Western countries. Among the many forces that contribute to body dissatisfaction, the overrepresentation of thin bodies in visual media has received notable attention. In this study, we proposed that prevalence-induced concept change may be one of the cognitive mechanisms that explain how beauty standards shift. We conducted a preregistered online experiment with young women (N = 419) and found that when the prevalence of thin bodies in the environment increased, the concept of being overweight expanded to include bodies that would otherwise be judged as “normal.” Exploratory analyses revealed significant individual differences in sensitivity to this effect, in terms of women’s judgments about other bodies as well as their own. These results suggest that women’s judgments about other women’s bodies are biased by an overrepresentation of thinness and lend initial support to policies designed to increase size-inclusive representation in the media.
Abstract. Most feedback we receive or give is correct (deterministic
feedback), though a small fraction can be wrong for various reasons. Children need to cope
with receiving some portion of wrong feedback (stochastic feedback). It is still unknown
if better social functioning and communication skills or outstanding intelligence (IQ) or
chronological age support children in the coping process. We tested a sample of
7-, 9-, and 11-year-old children (N = 60) who deduced a
sequence of four left and right button presses from a red and green stochastic feedback
signal that was wrong in 15 % of the trials. Children performed worse with
stochastic than with deterministic feedback but improved in the repeated trials,
especially after receiving positive feedback about whether true or false. Controlling for
IQ improved and confirmed these effects, while social and communicative competence
explained little or no variance.
Zusammenfassung. Die Vereinbarkeit von Wissenschaft und Familie ist für Eltern im
Allgemeinen und Frauen im Besonderen eine große Herausforderung. Tagungsteilnahmen sind wichtige
Karrierebausteine und eine organisatorische Herausforderung für Eltern. In diesem Positionspapier wird
ein Stimmungsbild zu familienfreundlicheren Kongressgestaltung in der Fachgruppe (FG) Klinische Psychologie
und Psychotherapie der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie (DGPs) erfragt. 147 FG-Mitglieder
(Rücklaufquote: 18.36 %) beantworteten Fragen zur Demographie, sowie Einstellungen gegenüber
der FG-Tagung, Betreuungsmöglichkeiten und familienfreundlichen Maßnahmen. Von den Teilnehmenden
waren 66 % Eltern, 45 % sagten wegen familiärer Verpflichtungen die FG-Tagung ab.
Zusätzliche Kosten durch familiäre Verpflichtungen wurden als hoch eingeschätzt und
familienfreundlichere Maßnahmen von vielen Teilnehmenden gewünscht. Familienfreundliche Konferenzen
können ein klares Signal der Inklusion und Solidarität setzen und für die Aufrechterhaltung und
Nachhaltigkeit wissenschaftlicher Kompetenz sorgen. Konkrete Empfehlungen für eine familienfreundliche
Konferenzgestaltung werden als Checkliste im elektronischen Anhang zur Verfügung gestellt.
Background
Longitudinal observational studies play on an important role for evidence-based research on health services and psychiatric rehabilitation. However, information is missing about the reasons, why patients participate in such studies, and how they evaluate their participation experience.
Methods
Subsequently to their final assessment in a 2-year follow-up study on supported housing for persons with severe mental illness, n = 182 patients answered a short questionnaire on their study participation experience (prior experiences, participation reasons, burden due to study assessments, intention to participate in studies again). Basic respondent characteristics as well as symptom severity (SCL-K9) were also included in the descriptive and analytical statistics.
Results
To help other people and curiosity were cited as the main initial reasons for study participation (>85%). Further motives were significantly associated with demographic and/or clinical variables. For instance, “relieve from boredom” was more frequently reported by men and patients with substance use disorders (compared to mood disorders), and participants ‘motive” to talk about illness” was associated with higher symptom severity at study entry. Furthermore, only a small proportion of respondents indicated significant burdens by study participation and about 87% would also participate in future studies.
Conclusions
The respondents gave an overall positive evaluation regarding their participation experience in an observational study on psychiatric rehabilitation. The results additionally suggest that health and social care professionals should be responsive to the expectations and needs of patients with mental illness regarding participation in research.
Given the increasing prevalence of chronic kidney disease (CKD) and its impact on health care, it is important to better understand the multiple factors influencing health-related quality of life (HRQOL), particularly since they have been shown to affect CKD outcomes. Determinants of HRQOL as measured by the validated Kidney Disease Quality of Life questionnaire (KDQOL) and the Patient Health Questionnaire depression screener (PHQ-9) were assessed in a routine CKD patient sample, the Greifswald Approach to Individualized Medicine (GANI_MED) renal cohort (N = 160), including a wide range of self-reported data, sociodemographic and laboratory measures. Compared to the general population, CKD patients had lower HRQOL indices. Dialysis was associated with (1) low levels of physical functioning, (2) increased impairments by symptoms and problems, and (3) more effects and burden of kidney disease. HRQOL is seriously affected in CKD patients. However, impairments were found irrespective of eGFR decline and albuminuria. Rather, the comorbid conditions of depression and diabetes predicted a lower HRQOL (physical component score). Further studies should address whether recognizing and treating depression may not only improve HRQOL but also promote survival and lower hospitalization rates of CKD patients.
Loneliness and lack of belonging as paramount theme in identity descriptions of Children Born of War
(2022)
Objective
Children Born of War (CBOW) are an international and timeless phenomenon that exists in every country involved in war or armed conflict. Nevertheless, little is known on a systematic level about those children, who are typically fathered by a foreign or enemy soldier and born to a local mother. In particular, the identity issues that CBOW often report have remained largely uninvestigated. In the current qualitative study we began filling this gap in the scientific literature by asking how CBOW construct their identity in self-descriptions.
Method
We utilized thematic content analysis of N = 122 German CBOWs' answers to an open-ended questionnaire item asking how they see themselves and their identity in the context of being a CBOW.
Results
We identified five key themes in CBOW' identity accounts. Loneliness and lack of belonging appeared as a paramount aspect of their self-descriptions next to narratives about belonging and positive relationship. On a less interpersonal basis, we found fighting and surviving and searching for truth and completion overarching aspects of their identities. There were also few accounts growing up unaffected by the fact of being born a CBOW. Although all themes portray different perspectives, they all (but the last one) clearly indicate the impeded circumstances under which CBOW had to grow up.
Conclusions
Integrating our findings with existing interdisciplinary literature regarding identity, we discuss implications for future research and clinical and political practice.
Many orally dosed APIs are bioavailable only when formulated as an enteric dosage form to protect them from the harsh environment of the stomach. However, an enteric formulation is often accompanied with a higher development effort in the first place and the potential degradation of fragile APIs during the coating process. Ready-to-use enteric hard capsules would be an easily available alternative to test and develop APIs in enteric formulations, while decreasing the time and cost of process development. In this regard, Lonza Capsugel® Next Generation Enteric capsules offer a promising approach as functional capsules. The in vivo performance of these capsules was observed with two independent techniques (MRI and caffeine in saliva) in eight human volunteers. No disintegration or content release in the stomach was observed, even after highly variable individual gastric residence times (range 7.5 to 82.5 min), indicating the reliable enteric properties of these capsules. Seven capsules disintegrated in the distal part of the small intestine; one capsule showed an uncommonly fast intestinal transit (15 min) and disintegrated in the colon. The results for this latter capsule by MRI and caffeine appearance differed dramatically, whereas for all other capsules disintegrating in the small intestine, the results were very comparable, which highlights the necessity for reliable and complementary measurement methods. No correlation could be found between the gastric residence time and disintegration after gastric emptying, which confirms the robust enteric formulation of those capsules.
Quality of Life (QoL) is a core patient-reported outcome in healthcare research. However, a conceptual, operational, and psychometric elaboration of QoL in the context of telemedical care (TM) was needed, as standardised instruments to assess QoL do not comprehensively represent essential aspects of intended outcomes of TM. Therefore, the overall aim of this thesis was to conceptualise QoL in the context of TM and to develop an instrument that can adequately assess QoL in TM.
The COVID-19 pandemic is one of the most serious health and economic crises of the 21st century. From a psychological point of view, the COVID-19 pandemic and its consequences can be conceptualized as a multidimensional and potentially toxic stressor for mental health in the general population. This selective literature review provides an overview of longitudinal studies published until June 2021 that have investigated the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the European population. Risk and protective factors identified in the studies are summarized. Forty-two studies that met inclusion and search criteria (COVID-19, mental health, longitudinal, and Europe) in PubMed, PsycInfo, and Web of Science databases indicate differential effects of the pandemic on mental distress, depression, and anxiety, depending on samples and methods used. Age-specific (e.g., young age), social (e.g., female, ethnical minority, loneliness), as well as physical and mental health-related factors (e.g., pre-pandemic illness) were identified as risk factors for poor mental health. The studies point to several protective factors such as social support, higher cognitive ability, resilience, and self-efficacy. Increasing evidence supports the assumption of the pandemic being a multidimensional stressor on mental health, with some populations appearing more vulnerable than others, although inconsistencies arise. Whether the pandemic will lead to an increase in the prevalence of mental disorders is an open question. Further high-quality longitudinal and multi-national studies and meta-analyses are needed to draw the complete picture of the consequences of the pandemic on mental health.
This review assessed population-based estimate rates of cancer patients with minor and young adult children (≤ 25 years), children and young adults having a parent with cancer as well as the psychosocial situation and well-being of children and young adults affected by parental cancer. Eighteen publications on population-based studies were included. Studies varied in the age ranges of both cancer patients and children. The prevalence rates of cancer patients having children ranged from 14 to 24.7% depending on the sample structure (e.g., age, gender). Studies reported that between 1.6 and 8.4% of children resp. young adult children have a parent with a history of cancer. Seven publications reported on the psychosocial situation or well-being in children and young adults affected by parental cancer. Estimate rates of psychosocial problems, psychiatric diagnoses or distress ranged between 2.5 and 34% of children depending on the method of measurement and outcome. The differences in the sample structure between the studies impeded the comparison of prevalence rates. However, the findings help to determine the need for specific support services and health care planning. The results emphazise the importance to routinely include issues on the parental role of patients and questions on the well-being and coping of children into psychooncological care. If necessary, support should be provided to families living with a cancer diagnosis.
Functional connectivity studies have demonstrated that creative thinking builds upon an interplay of multiple neural networks involving the cognitive control system. Theoretically, cognitive control has generally been discussed as the common basis underlying the positive relationship between creative thinking and intelligence. However, the literature still lacks a detailed investigation of the association patterns between cognitive control, the factors of creative thinking as measured by divergent thinking (DT) tasks, i.e., fluency and originality, and intelligence, both fluid and crystallized. In the present study, we explored these relationships at the behavioral and the neural level, based on N = 77 young adults. We focused on brain-signal complexity (BSC), parameterized by multi-scale entropy (MSE), as measured during a verbal DT and a cognitive control task. We demonstrated that MSE is a sensitive neural indicator of originality as well as inhibition. Then, we explore the relationships between MSE and factor scores indicating DT and intelligence. In a series of across-scalp analyses, we show that the overall MSE measured during a DT task, as well as MSE measured in cognitive control states, are associated with fluency and originality at specific scalp locations, but not with fluid and crystallized intelligence. The present explorative study broadens our understanding of the relationship between creative thinking, intelligence, and cognitive control from the perspective of BSC and has the potential to inspire future BSC-related theories of creative thinking.
Fear is an emotional state, characterized by the activation of a defense system that is designed to ensure the organism’s survival. This system enables a rapid recognition of threats and organizes defensive response patterns in order to adaptively cope with the threatening environment. Yet, to ensure its flexibility under changing environmental conditions, inhibitory pathways exist that modulate the activation of this defense system, if a previously threatening cue no longer predicts any harm – a memory-formatting process referred to as fear extinction, leading to a reduction of defensive responding. Fear extinction is presumed to at least partially underlie exposure treatment of anxiety disorders, which is why the facilitation of this learning process may promote such treatment’s efficacy. Animal models suggested, that the stimulation of the vagus nerve or the superior colliculus (SC) – a midbrain structure mediating visual attentional processing – target these inhibitory extinction pathways and, thus, facilitate fear extinction. However, as it is unclear whether similar mechanisms exist in humans, this thesis manuscript examined how non-invasive stimulation of these inhibitory pathways by transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) or SC-recruiting visual attentional manipulation impact on human fear extinction.
To this end, we conducted three studies using multiple-day single-cue fear conditioning and extinction paradigms. First, we elaborated on fear that is established in these paradigms by examining defensive responding that is elicited by an innocuous conditioned stimulus, which has either been paired (fear learning group) with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; an electric shock) or was unpaired (control group; study 1). During the following extinction training, either tVNS vs. sham stimulation was applied (study 1, study 2) or participants were instructed, to either generate saccadic eye movements (strong SC activation) vs. smooth eye pursuits (low SC activation; study 3). During subsequent sessions, extinction consolidation as well as the short- and long-term extinction recall was tested (study 2, study 3).
Conditioned fear in the fear learning group was characterized by elevated cognitive risk assessments (US-expectancy ratings), as well as increased cardiac deceleration and startle reflex potentiation compared to controls. Cardiac deceleration was positively correlated to startle potentiation, but was decoupled from cognitive risk assessments (study 1). Initial, short- and long-term extinction of these defensive responses was facilitated by tVNS on all three response levels (cognitive, physiological, behavioral; study 1, study 2). In contrast, saccades facilitated initial extinction only for physiological and behavioral elements of the defensive response pattern, while extinction consolidation and recall was impaired by any eye movement manipulation (study 3) for physiological and behavioral indicators of defensive responding.
Taken together, the data of the experimental series suggest, that on a behavioral level, conditioned fear may best be conceived as attentive immobility – a defense strategy elicited by inevitable distal threats, that is uniformly expressed across species and is accompanied by cardiac deceleration and startle reflex potentiation. In addition, it was shown that such rather automatic defensive adaptations are independent from verbally expressed threat expectancies. As expected, tVNS impacted on fear extinction on both levels, strongly in line with the suggestion, that vagal stimulation activates cortical and subcortical neural pathways involved in extinction learning, consolidation and recall. TVNS may, thus, be a promising adjuvant for exposure treatment of mental disorders. In contrast, SC-recruiting visual attentional manipulation only affected subcortically mediated defensive responding, in line with rodent findings, indicating that the SC specifically inhibits subcortical parts of the neural defense system. However, as extinction recall was impaired by any type of visual attentional manipulation, this appeared to have functioned as a form of avoidance, initially attenuating fear but preventing extinction consolidation and, thus, impairing sustained fear reduction. Both non-invasive stimulation techniques may therefore increase initial defensive flexibility in the face of no-longer threat-signaling stimuli, but only tVNS may achieve long-term effects on multiple response levels.
Noninvasive Modulation of Cognition in Older Adults – Neural Correlates and Behavioral Outcomes
(2022)
Background: The worldwide population will grow older in the upcoming years. Aging, even in the absence of pathological processes, is associated with decline in cognitive functioning. Age-related cognitive decline is not a uniform process affecting every function in the same way. Research should thus examine specific functions, such as episodic memory or executive functions differentially, especially in older samples. Neural correlates of these functions are well characterized in young adults. However, research on functional and structural neural substrates underlying specific cognitive functions in older adults is scarce, but needed for example to identify targets for noninvasive interventions against cognitive decline. Current advances in this field point towards the effectiveness of combined noninvasive interventions of cognitive training and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). So far, evidence regarding such combined interventions in older adults is inconclusive and neural mechanisms of successful interventions are still unclear. This line of research could advance the development of noninvasive methods to modulate cognitive functions and therefore address the unmet need for treatment options against age-related cognitive decline.
Aims: The main aims of the present thesis were (i) to characterize functional and structural neural network correlates of two cognitive functions with high relevance for daily living, namely working memory updating and value-based decision making, (ii) to map out interventions of combined cognitive training and tDCS to modulate these cognitive functions in healthy older adults and adults with prodromal AD and (iii) to assess behavioral and neural outcomes of combined cognitive training (of either executive functions or visuo-spatial memory) and tDCS interventions.
Methods: In order to address these aims, the present thesis includes five papers. Paper I assessed functional and structural neural correlates of working memory updating and value-based decision-making performance in healthy older adults using functional magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging. Papers II and III comprise study protocols of a three-week cognitive training of working memory updating and value-based decision-making abilities with concurrent tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy older adults and adults with prodromal AD. Paper IV used mixed-model analysis to compute behavioral outcomes of this combined intervention in healthy older adults, including outcome measures of the trained tasks, measures of transfer to untrained tasks and assessment of long-term effects at four weeks and half a year after the intervention. In paper V neural alterations after cognitive training of visuo-spatial memory and active stimulation compared to the training and sham (placebo) stimulation were assessed in older adults using measures of functional network centrality and diffusion tensor imaging-derived measures of grey matter microstructure.
Results: Analyses revealed distinct functional and structural connectivity correlates of performance on the two cognitive tasks assessing working memory updating and value-based decision-making (paper I). Moreover, results showed a combined contribution of a specific white matter pathway (cingulum bundle) and frontoparietal functional connectivity to working memory updating performance, thereby providing information on possible network targets for modulation of working memory updating and value-based decision-making. The combined modulatory intervention of cognitive training and tDCS (papers II and IV) did not demonstrate group differences between concurrent active tDCS over sham tDCS in the trained tasks. However, analysis of a working memory transfer task revealed a beneficial effect of training and active tDCS over training and sham tDCS at post intervention and follow-up. Findings from paper V showed reduced functional network centrality after visuo-spatial memory training and active tDCS compared to training and sham tDCS in the stimulated brain area and its contralateral homologue. Additionally, after the training, measures of grey matter microstructural plasticity in the stimulated brain area were associated with beneficial training outcomes for the active but not the sham stimulation group.
Conclusion: Taken together, cognitive performance, functional and structural networks as well as the possibility of their modulation through combined cognitive training and tDCS interventions were investigated in older adults with and without cognitive impairment. The present thesis therefore contributes to the field of noninvasive neuromodulation in older adults by characterizing distinct neural correlates of two age-sensitive executive functions, which may be altered through modulatory interventions. Moreover, the present work shows that cognitive training with concurrent tDCS holds the potential to elicit transfer effects to untrained tasks and further may evoke neural plasticity on the functional and microstructural level. This work thus promotes the development of modulatory interventions against age-associated cognitive decline and holds promising implications for translation to clinical application.
Ungeachtet der Alltagsrelevanz ist die empirische Evidenz zur Regulation selbstbewer-tender Emotionen im Forschungsfeld unterrepräsentiert. In Dual-Process-Ansätzen wurden Zusammenhänge zwischen Emotionen, kognitiver Emotionsregulation und Entscheidungs-verhalten in moralischen Konflikten modelliert. Jüngere Befunde legen nahe, dass habituelles und experimentell induziertes Reappraisal – mediiert über die emotionale Erregung – positiv mit konsequentialistischen Urteilen und Entscheidungen assoziiert sind.
Ziel der Arbeit ist es, den Einfluss kognitiver Emotionsregulation auf das Entscheidungs-verhalten in moralischen Alltagsdilemmata zu untersuchen. Welche kognitiven Strategien kommen bei der Schuld- und Schamregulation zum Einsatz? Welche Wirkung entfalten sie auf verschiedene Outcomes (emotionales Erleben, Entscheidungsverhalten)? Inwiefern unterscheiden sich Formen und Taktiken des Reappraisal in ihrer Wirkung?
In einem ersten Schritt wurden schuld- und schamauslösende Dilemmata entwickelt und anhand definierter Kriterien selektiert. Eine Studienreihe betrachtete den Einfluss habitueller, kognitiver Emotionsregulation und experimentell manipuliertem Reappraisal auf das Entscheidungsverhalten in diesen Dilemmata. Tendenziell begünstigten funktionale Strategien aus der Reappraisal-Familie konsequentialistische Entscheidungen. Der Media-tionseffekt über die emotionale Erregung konnte nicht repliziert werden. Eine zweite Studien-reihe mit explorativer Methodik beabsichtigte, die Phänomenologie von Reappraisal-Taktiken bei einem moralischen Entscheidungskonflikt abzubilden. Mittels eines Kategoriensystems konnten problemorientierte und externalisierende Reappraisal-Taktiken identifiziert werden.
Limitationen der Untersuchungen und Anknüpfungspunkte für zukünftige Forschung werden ebenso diskutiert wie Implikationen der Ergebnisse für die klinische und forensische Praxis.
Quality of Life in Young Adults With Cerebral Palsy: A Longitudinal Analysis of the SPARCLE Study
(2021)
Introduction: While most people with cerebral palsy (CP) will have a life expectancy similar to that of the general population, international research has primarily focused on childhood and adolescence; and knowledge about the quality of life (QoL) of young adults with CP, its trajectories, and associated factors remains scarce.
Methods: This longitudinal study included young adults with CP living in five European regions and who had previously participated in the SPARCLE cohort as children and/or adolescents. Their QoL in the psychological well-being and social relationships domains was estimated using age-appropriate validated instruments (KIDSCREEN-52 in childhood/adolescence and WHOQOL-Bref in young adulthood). We used generalized linear mixed-effect models with random intercept to estimate long-term trajectories of QoL in both domains and to investigate whether severity of impairment, pain, and seizure influenced these trajectories. We sought to identify potentially different trajectories of QoL from childhood to adulthood using a shape-based clustering method.
Results: In total, 164 young adults with CP aged 22–27 years participated in the study. Psychological well-being linearly decreased by 0.78 points (scale 0–100) per year (95% confidence interval (CI) −0.99 to −0.56) from childhood to young adulthood, whereas QoL in the social relationships domain increased (β coefficient 1.24, 95% CI 0.92–1.55). Severity of impairment was associated with reduced QoL in all life periods of the study (childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood): motor impairment with social relationships, and more nuancedly intellectual disability with psychological well-being and social relationships. At all periods, frequent pain significantly reduced psychological well-being, and seizures were associated with lower QoL in the social relationships domain. In both domains, we identified a group of individuals with CP who presented a reverse trajectory compared with the general QoL trajectory.
Conclusion: Identification of QoL trajectories and their associated factors yields improved knowledge about the experience of individuals with CP until young adulthood. Further studies are needed to better understand the determinants that have the greatest influence on the different shapes of long-term trajectories of QoL.
Quality of life (QoL) is a core patient-reported outcome in healthcare research, alongside primary clinical outcomes. A conceptual, operational, and psychometric elaboration of QoL in the context of TM is needed, because standardized instruments to assess QoL do not sufficiently represent essential aspects of intended outcomes of telemedical applications (TM). The overall aim is to develop an instrument that can adequately capture QoL in TM. For that purpose, an extended working model of QoL will be derived. Subsequently, an instrument will be developed and validated that captures those aspects of QoL that are influenced by TM. The initial exploratory study section includes (a) a systematic literature review, (b) a qualitative survey for concept elicitation, and (c) pre-testings using cognitive debriefings with patients and an expert workshop. The second quantitative section consists of an online expert survey and two patient surveys for piloting and validation of the newly developed instrument. The resulting questionnaire will assess central experiences of patients regarding telemedical applications and its impact on QoL more sensitively. Its use as adjunct instrument will lead to a more appropriate evaluation of TM and contribute to the improvement of care tailored to patients’ individual needs.
Moral rules are a cornerstone of many societies. Most moral rules are concerned with the welfare of other individuals, reflecting individuals’ innate aversion against harming other individuals. Harming others is associated with aversive experiences, implying that individuals who are sensitive to the aversiveness of these experiences are more likely to follow moral rules than individuals who are insensitive to the aversiveness of these experiences. Individuals’ sensitivity for aversive experiences depends on individuals’ ability to integrate the underlying neural and physiological processes: Individuals who are more efficient in integrating these processes are more sensitive to the aversiveness that is associated with moral rule violations than individuals who are less efficient in integrating these processes. Individuals who differ in their ability to integrate these processes may, thus, also differ in their inclination to follow moral rules. We tested this assumption in a sample of healthy individuals (67 males) who completed measures of moral rule adherence and integration abilities. Moral rule adherence was assessed with self-report measure and integration abilities were assessed with a resting state measure of heart rate variability (HRV), which reflects prefrontal–(para-)limbic engagement during the integration of physical and neural processes. We found a positive association between individuals’ HRV and individuals’ moral rule adherence, implying that individuals with efficient integration abilities were more inclined to follow moral rules than individuals with inefficient integration abilities. Our findings support the assumption that individuals with different integration abilities also differ in moral rule adherence, presumably because of differences in aversiveness sensitivity.
Background: There is an urgent need for effective follow-up treatments after acute electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) in depressed patients. Preliminary evidence suggests psychotherapeutic interventions to be a feasible and efficacious follow-up treatment. However, there is a need for research on the long-term usefulness of such psychotherapeutic offers in a naturalistic setting that is more representative of routine clinical practice. Therefore, the aim of the current pilot study was to investigate the effects of a half-open continuous group cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) with cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy elements as a follow-up treatment for all ECT patients, regardless of response status after ECT, on reducing depressive symptoms and promoting psychosocial functioning.
Method: Group CBT was designed to support patients during the often-difficult transition from inpatient to outpatient treatment. In a non-controlled pilot trial, patients were offered 15weekly sessions of manualized group CBT (called EffECTiv 2.0). The Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale was assessed as primary outcome; the Beck Depression Inventory, WHO Quality of Life Questionnaire–BREF, and the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire were assessed as secondary outcomes. Measurements took place before individual group start, after individual group end, and 6months after individual group end.
Results: During group CBT, Post-ECT symptom reduction was not only maintained but there was a tendency toward a further decrease in depression severity. This reduction could be sustained 6months after end of the group, regardless of response status after ECT treatment. Aspects of quality of life and emotion regulation strategies improved during group CBT, and these improvements were maintained 6months after the end of the group.
Conclusion: Even though the interpretability of the results is limited by the small sample and the non-controlled design, they indicate that manualized group CBT with cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy elements might pose a recommendable follow-up treatment option after acute ECT for depressed patients, regardless of response status after ECT. This approach might not only help to further reduce depressive symptoms and prevent relapse, but also promote long-term psychosocial functioning by improving emotion regulation strategies and psychological quality of life and thus could be considered as a valuable addition to clinical routine after future validation.
Background: Interpersonal skills deficits and dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs have been implicated in the etiology and maintenance of depression. This study aimed to investigate the association between changes in these skills deficits and change in depressive symptoms over the course of treatment with Cognitive Behavioral Analysis System of Psychotherapy (CBASP) and Metacognitive Therapy (MCT).
Methods: In this prospective, parallel group observational study, data was collected at baseline and after 8 weeks of an intensive day clinic psychotherapy program. Based on a shared decision between patients and clinicians, patients received either CBASP or MCT. Ninety patients were included in the analyses (CBASP: age M = 38.7, 40.5% female, MCT: age M = 44.7, 43.3% female). Interpersonal deficits were assessed with the short-form of the Luebeck Questionnaire for Recording Preoperational Thinking (LQPT-SF) and the Impact Message Inventory (IMI-R). Metacognitive beliefs were assessed with the Metacognition Questionnaire-30 (MCQ-30). The Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS-SR16) was utilized to assess depressive symptoms. A regression analysis was conducted to assess variables associated with outcome. ANCOVAs were utilized to investigate whether improvement in skills deficits is dependent on type of treatment received.
Results: Improvements in preoperational thinking and increases in friendly-dominant behavior were associated with change in depressive symptoms. There was no association between reductions in dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs and a decrease in depressive symptoms. While both treatment groups showed significant improvements in interpersonal and metacognitive skills, there was no significant between-group difference in the change scores for either of these skills.
Conclusion: Our findings suggest that changes in interpersonal skills seem to be of particular relevance in the treatment of depression. These results have to be replicated in a randomized-controlled design before firm conclusions can be drawn.
Zusammenfassung. Die Klinische Psychologie ist essentieller Bestandteil interdisziplinärer
Forschung im Bereich psychischer Störungen. Ziel der präsentierten Analyse war es, den individuellen
Beitrag der Psychologie abzubilden. Wir bestimmten daher objektive Indikatoren für die Mitwirkung der
deutschsprachigen Klinischen Psychologie und ihrer psychologischen Nachbardisziplinen an den Forschungsthemen
zu psychischen Störungen. Eine themenbezogene Drittmittelanalyse für den Zeitraum
2000 – 2018 identifizierte 85 Großprojekte unter klinisch-psychologischer
Leitung sowie 10 Personenförderungen mit einem Gesamtfördervolumen von 156 Millionen Euro.
Weiterhin zeigte eine Publikationsanalyse, dass in den Jahren 1980 – 2018 die
150 deutschsprachigen Autor_innen mit den meisten themenrelevanten Publikationen mehrheitlich
(63 %) einen akademischen Abschluss in Psychologie hatten. Die Anzahl der Publikationen nahm unter den
psychologischen Autor_innen insgesamt jährlich zu. Wir identifizierten ein breites Forschungsfeld in
störungsspezifischer, verfahrensspezifischer und neurowissenschaftlich fundierter Forschung. Die
Ergebnisse verdeutlichen den substantiellen Beitrag der Psychologie in der Grundlagen- und
Behandlungsforschung zu psychischen Störungen.
Zusammenfassung: Die Klinische Psychologie ist essentieller Bestandteil
interdisziplinärer Forschung im Bereich psychischer Störungen. Ziel der
präsentierten Analyse war es, den individuellen Beitrag der Psychologie abzubilden.
Wir bestimmten daher objektive Indikatoren für die Mitwirkung der deutschsprachigen
Klinischen Psychologie und ihrer psychologischen Nachbardisziplinen an den
Forschungsthemen zu psychischen Störungen. Eine themenbezogene Drittmittelanalyse
für den Zeitraum 2000 – 2018 identifizierte
85 Großprojekte unter klinisch-psychologischer Leitung sowie
10 Personenförderungen mit einem Gesamtfördervolumen von 156 Millionen
Euro. Weiterhin zeigte eine Publikationsanalyse, dass in den Jahren
1980 – 2018 die 150 deutschsprachigen Autor_innen mit den
meisten themenrelevanten Publikationen mehrheitlich (63 %) einen akademischen
Abschluss in Psychologie hatten. Die Anzahl der Publikationen nahm unter den
psychologischen Autor_innen insgesamt jährlich zu. Wir identifizierten ein breites
Forschungsfeld in störungsspezifischer, verfahrensspezifischer und
neurowissenschaftlich fundierter Forschung. Die Ergebnisse verdeutlichen den
substantiellen Beitrag der Psychologie in der Grundlagen- und Behandlungsforschung zu
psychischen Störungen.
Background: Depression is a highly prevalent mental disorder, but only a fraction of those affected receive evidence-based treatments. Recently, Internet-based interventions were introduced as an efficacious and cost-effective approach. However, even though depression is a heterogenous construct, effects of treatments have mostly been determined using aggregated symptom scores. This carries the risk of concealing important effects and working mechanisms of those treatments.
Methods: In this study, we analyze outcome and long-term follow-up data from the EVIDENT study, a large (N = 1,013) randomized-controlled trial comparing an Internet intervention for depression (Deprexis) with care as usual. We use Network Intervention Analysis to examine the symptom-specific effects of the intervention. Using data from intermediary and long-term assessments that have been conducted over 36 months, we intend to reveal how the treatment effects unfold sequentially and are maintained.
Results: Item-level analysis showed that scale-level effects can be explained by small item-level effects on most depressive symptoms at all points of assessment. Higher scores on these items at baseline predicted overall symptom reduction throughout the whole assessment period. Network intervention analysis offered insights into potential working mechanisms: while deprexis directly affected certain symptoms of depression (e.g., worthlessness and fatigue) and certain aspects of the quality of life (e.g., overall impairment through emotional problems), other domains were affected indirectly (e.g., depressed mood and concentration as well as activity level). The configuration of direct and indirect effects replicates previous findings from another study examining the same intervention.
Conclusions: Internet interventions for depression are not only effective in the short term, but also exert long-term effects. Their effects are likely to affect only a small subset of problems. Patients reporting these problems are likely to benefit more from the intervention. Future studies on online interventions should examine symptom-specific effects as they potentially reveal the potential of treatment tailoring.
Clinical Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT02178631.
“Blood for Blood”? Personal Motives and Deterrents for Blood Donation in the German Population
(2021)
Over the last decades, various predictors have proven relevant for job performance [e.g., general mental ability (GMA), broad personality traits, such as the Big Five]. However, prediction of job performance is far from perfect, and further potentially relevant predictors need to be investigated. Narrower personality traits, such as individuals' character strengths, have emerged as meaningfully related to different aspects of job performance. However, it is still unclear whether character strengths can explain additional variance in job performance over and above already known powerful predictors. Consequently, the present study aimed at (1) examining the incremental validity of character strengths as predictors of job performance beyond GMA and/or the Big Five traits and (2) identifying the most important predictors of job performance out of the 24 character strengths, GMA, and the Big Five. Job performance was operationalized with multidimensional measures of both productive and counterproductive work behavior. A sample of 169 employees from different occupations completed web-based self-assessments on character strengths, GMA, and the Big Five. Additionally, the employees' supervisors provided web-based ratings of their job performance. Results showed that character strengths incrementally predicted job performance beyond GMA, the Big Five, or GMA plus the Big Five; explained variance increased up to 54.8, 43.1, and 38.4%, respectively, depending on the dimension of job performance. Exploratory relative weight analyses revealed that for each of the dimensions of job performance, at least one character strength explained a numerically higher amount of variance than GMA and the Big Five, except for individual task proactivity, where GMA exhibited the numerically highest amount of explained variance. The present study shows that character strengths are relevant predictors of job performance in addition to GMA and other conceptualizations of personality (i.e., the Big Five). This also highlights the role of socio-emotional skills, such as character strengths, for the understanding of performance outcomes above and beyond cognitive ability.
Introduction: The Cognitive Behavioral Analysis System of Psychotherapy (CBASP) was developed for the treatment of persistent depressive disorder (PDD), where comorbid personality disorders (PD) are common. In contrast to other PD, comorbid borderline personality disorder (BPD) is often regarded as an exclusion criterion for CBASP. In clinical settings, however, subthreshold BPD symptoms are prevalent in PDD and may not be obvious at an initial assessment prior to therapy. As data on their impact on CBASP outcome are very limited, this naturalistic study investigates BPD features in PDD and their relevance for the therapeutic outcome of a multimodal CBASP inpatient program.
Method: Sixty patients (37 female, mean age 38.3, SD 11.9 years) meeting DSM-5 criteria for PDD underwent a 10 weeks CBASP inpatient program. BPD features (i.e., number of fulfilled DSM-5 criteria) together with childhood maltreatment and rejection sensitivity were assessed on admission. Before and after treatment, severity of depressive symptoms was measured using the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). BPD symptoms were assessed using the Borderline Personality Disorder Severity Index (BPDSI-IV) and the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23). Intercorrelations of baseline characteristics and symptom change during treatment were analyzed.
Results: Patients with PDD met a mean of 1.5 (SD 1.6) BPD criteria with 4 patients fulfilling ≥5 criteria. BPD symptoms and depressive symptoms showed a strong correlation, and BPD symptoms were additionally correlated with emotional abuse and rejection sensitivity. There was no association between BPD features at baseline and improvement on the MADRS, however, BPD features tended to be associated with a lower response according to the BDI-II score after 10 weeks of treatment. Furthermore, BPD symptoms (i.e., abandonment, impulsivity and affective instability) were reduced after 10 weeks of CBASP treatment.
Discussion: BPD symptoms are prevalent in patients with PDD and highly intertwined with the experience of depressive symptoms. In this naturalistic study in PDD, BPD features at baseline did not limit the clinical response to CBASP. Future studies may extend the spectrum of PDD to comorbid subsyndromal or even syndromal BPD in order to develop tailored psychotherapeutic treatment for these complex affective disorders.
Abstract
Background
Comorbidities in mental disorders are often understood by assuming a common cause. The network theory of mental disorders offers an alternative to this assumption by understanding comorbidities as mutually reinforced problems. In this study, we used network analysis to examine bridge symptoms between anxiety and depression in a large sample.
Method
Using data from a sample of patients diagnosed with both depression and an anxiety disorder before and after inpatient treatment (N = 5,614, mean age: 42.24, 63.59% female, average treatment duration: 48.12 days), network models of depression and anxiety symptoms are estimated. Topology, the centrality of nodes, stability, and changes in network structure are analyzed. Symptoms that drive comorbidity are determined by bridge node analysis. As an alternative to network communities based on categorical diagnosis, we performed a community analysis and propose empirically derived symptom subsets.
Results
The obtained network models are highly stable. Sad mood and the inability to control worry are the most central. Psychomotor agitation or retardation is the strongest bridge node between anxiety and depression, followed by concentration problems and restlessness. Changes in appetite and suicidality were unique to depression. Community analysis revealed four symptom groups.
Conclusion
The estimated network structure of depression and anxiety symptoms proves to be highly accurate. Results indicate that some symptoms are considerably more influential than others and that only a small number of predominantly physical symptoms are strong candidates for explaining comorbidity. Future studies should include physiological measures in network models to provide a more accurate understanding.
Körpersignale sind elementar für die Aufrechterhaltung der Homöostase, um eine angemessene Regulation der Körperfunktionen zu ermöglichen und dadurch das Überleben des Individuums sicherzustellen. Die bisherige Forschung hat die dysfunktionale Wahrnehmung dieser interozeptiven Signale als wichtigen Bestandteil vieler Angst- und Gesundheitsprobleme identifiziert, da selbst leichte und harmlose Körperempfindungen eine übermäßig starke Mobilisierung von Abwehrreaktionen hervorrufen können. Obwohl das wissenschaftliche Interesse im Bereich Interozeption in den letzten Jahren stark gewachsen ist, wurde die Dynamik von Abwehrreaktionen als Reaktion auf Körpersignale bisher selten untersucht. Daher hatte die vorliegende Arbeit zum Ziel, die verhaltensbezogene und psychophysiologische Dynamik der defensiven Mobilisierung auf bedrohliche interozeptive Signale sowie den moderierenden Effekt von Dispositionsfaktoren und biologisch determinierten Verhaltensmarkern zu untersuchen.
In der ersten Studie wurden die Dynamik defensiver Mobilisierung auf eine näher-kommende externe Bedrohung mit einer sich annähernden interozeptiven respiratorischen Bedrohung, in Abhängigkeit von der Bedrohungsnähe und der Möglichkeit zur Vermeidung, bezüglich subjektiver, autonomer und respiratorische Reaktionen sowie Gehirnreaktionen und defensiver Reflexe verglichen.
In der zweiten Studie wurde die Mobilisierung defensiver Reaktionen während der wiederholten Vermeidung von kulminierender Atemnot analysiert, um eine detaillierte Analyse der Initiierung und Aufrechterhaltung von aktiven Vermeidungsverhalten zu erhalten.
Die dritte Studie unterweiterte die diese Befunde, indem die Rolle der maximalen freiwilligen Luftanhaltezeit als möglicher Prädiktor für eine übermäßig starke Mobilisierung defensiver Reaktionen bei der Konfrontation mit einer näherkommenden respiratorischen Bedrohung, in Abhängigkeit der Möglichkeit zur Vermeidung, untersucht wurde.
In der vierten Studie wurden die Prädiktoren für aktives Abwehrverhalten (d.h., Flucht oder aktive Vermeidung bei Konfrontation) während ansteigender Atemnot, induziert durch in der Intensität ansteigende inspiratorische Atemwiderstände gefolgt von einer kurzen Blockade der Einatmung, untersucht.
Die erste Studie zeigte, dass das defensive Aktivierungsmuster bei der Mobilisierung defensiver Reaktionen auf eine sich annähernde externe Bedrohung annähernd vergleichbar ist wie bei einer interozeptiven respiratorischen Bedrohung, unabhängig von der Möglichkeit zur Vermeidung. Zusätzlich wurde eine bedrohungsspezifische Mobilisierung des respiratorischen Systems bei der Konfrontation mit der unvermeidbaren interozeptiven Bedrohung beobachtet.
Die Daten der zweiten Studie demonstrierten, dass die Initiierung von erstmaligen Vermeidungsverhalten als Reaktion auf ansteigende Atemnot begleitet wird von physiologischen Erregungen als Indikatoren für eine Reaktionsvorbereitung. Diese verschwanden zunehmend mit wiederholter Vermeidung, was auf die Entwicklung von gewohnheitsmäßiger Vermeidung hindeutet.
Darüber hinaus wurde in der dritten Studie gezeigt, dass eine übermäßig starke Mobilisierung von Abwehrreaktionen auf eine sich annähernde unvermeidbare respiratorische Bedrohung durch eine reduzierte maximale freiwillige Luftanhaltezeit vorhergesagt wird, jedoch nicht, wenn die Möglichkeit zur Vermeidung vorhanden war.
Die vierte Studie demonstrierte, dass eine höhere Angstempfindlichkeit und eine kürzere freiwillige maximale Luftanhaltezeit mit aktivem Vermeidungsverhalten während ansteigender Atemnot assoziiert sind.
Zusammenfassend zeigen die vorliegenden Daten, dass die defensive Mobilisierung für eine exterozeptive Bedrohung vergleichbar ist wie für eine interozeptive Bedrohung und sich in Abhängigkeit der Bedrohungsnähe und dem verfügbarem Abwehrrepertoire ändert. Darüber hinaus ist die Neigung, Erregungsgefühle zu befürchten und eine verminderte Belastungstoleranz während freiwilligem Luftanhaltens mit einer erhöhten Angst- und Furchtreaktion auf Atemnotsymptome verbunden. Daher könnten diese dispositionellen und biologischen Verhaltensmarker bei der Konfrontation mit ansteigender Atemnot aktives Vermeidungsverhalten begünstigen und, wenn sie gewohnheitsmäßig ausgeführt werden, kann sich persistentes Vermeidungsverhalten entwickeln, welches das Risiko für die Entstehung einer ernsthaften psychischen Erkrankung erhöht.
Extinction learning is suggested to be a central mechanism during exposure-based cognitive behavioralpsychotherapy. A positive association between the patients’pretreatment extinction learning performance andtreatment outcome would corroborate the hypothesis. Indeed, there isfirst correlational evidence between reducedextinction learning and therapy efficacy. However, the results of these association studies may be hampered byextinction-training protocols that do not match treatment procedures. Therefore, we developed an extinction-trainingprotocol highly tailored to the procedure of exposure therapy and tested it in two samples of 46 subjects in total. Byusing instructed fear acquisition training, including a consolidation period overnight, we wanted to ensure that theconditioned fear response was well established prior to extinction training, which is the case in patients with anxietydisorders prior to treatment. Moreover, the extinction learning process was analyzed on multiple response levels,comprising unconditioned stimulus (US) expectancy ratings, autonomic responses, defensive brain stem reflexes, andneural activation using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Using this protocol, we found robust fearconditioning and slow-speed extinction learning. We also observed within-group heterogeneity in extinction learning,albeit a stable fear response at the beginning of the extinction training. Finally, we found discordance betweendifferent response systems, suggesting that multiple processes are involved in extinction learning. The paradigmpresented here might help to ameliorate the association between extinction learning performance assessed in thelaboratory and therapy outcomes and thus facilitate translational science in anxiety disorders
Development of the Greifswald questionnaire for the measurement of interprofessional attitudes
(2020)
Abstract
In crisis communication, warning messages are key to prevent or mitigate damage by informing the public about impending risks and hazards. The present study explored the influence of hazard type, trait anxiety, and warning message on different components of risk perception. A survey examined 614 German participants (18–96 years, M = 31.64, 63.0% female) using a pre–post comparison. Participants were randomly allocated to one of five hazards (severe weather, act of violence, breakdown of emergency number, discovery of a World War II bomb, or major fire) for which they received a warning message. Four components of risk perception (perceived severity, anticipatory worry, anticipated emotions, and perceived likelihood) were measured before and after the receipt. Also, trait anxiety was assessed. Analyses of covariance of risk perception were calculated, examining the effect of warning message, trait anxiety, and hazard type while controlling for age, gender, and previous hazard experience. Results showed main effects of hazard type and trait anxiety on every component of risk perception, except for perceived likelihood. The receipt of a warning message led to a significant decrease in anticipated negative emotions. However, changes across components of risk perception, as well as hazards, were inconsistent, as perceived severity decreased while perceived likelihood and anticipatory worry increased. In addition, three interactional effects were found (perceived severity × hazard type, perceived severity × trait anxiety, and anticipated emotions × hazard type). The findings point toward differences in the processing of warning messages yet underline the importance of hazard type, as well as characteristics of the recipient.
Our emotional experiences depend on our interoceptive ability to perceive and interpret changes in our autonomous nervous system. An inaccurate perception and interpretation of autonomic changes impairs our ability to understand and regulate our emotional reactions. Impairments in emotion understanding and emotion regulation increase our risk for mental disorders, indicating that interoceptive deficits play an important role in the etiology and pathogenesis of mental disorders. We, thus, need measures to identify those of us whose interoceptive deficits impair their emotion understanding and emotion regulation. Here, we used cardiac measures to investigate how our ability to engage prefrontal and (para-)limbic brain region regions affects our ability to perceive and interpret cardiac changes. We administered a heartbeat detection task to a sample of healthy individuals (n = 113) whose prefrontal-(para-) limbic engagement had been determined on basis of a heart rate variability recording. We found a positive association between heartbeat detection and heart rate variability, implying that individuals with higher heart rate variability were more accurate in heartbeat detection than individuals with lower heart rate variability. These findings suggest that our interoceptive accuracy depends on our prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement during the perception and interpretation of cardiac changes. Our findings also show that cardiac measures may be useful to investigate the association between interoceptive accuracy and prefrontal-(para-)limbic engagement in a time- and cost-efficient manner.
Introduction: Following behavioral recommendations is key to successful containment of the COVID-19 pandemic. Therefore, it is important to identify causes and patterns of non-compliance in the population to further optimize risk and health communication.
Methods: A total of 157 participants [80% female; mean age = 27.82 years (SD = 11.01)] were surveyed regarding their intention to comply with behavioral recommendations issued by the German government. Latent class analysis examined patterns of compliance, and subsequent multinomial logistic regression models tested sociodemographic (age, gender, country of origin, level of education, region, and number of persons per household) and psychosocial (knowledge about preventive behaviors, risk perception, stigmatizing attitudes) predictors.
Results: Three latent classes were identified: high compliance (25%) with all recommendations; public compliance (51%), with high compliance regarding public but not personal behaviors; and low compliance (24%) with most recommendations. Compared to high compliance, low compliance was associated with male gender [relative risk ratio (RRR) = 0.08 (0.01; 0.85)], younger age [RRR = 0.72 (0.57; 0.93)], and lower public stigma [RRR = 0.21 (0.05; 0.88)]. Low compliers were also younger than public compliers [RRR = 0.76 (0.59; 0.98)].
Discussion: With 25% of the sample reporting full compliance, and 51% differing in terms of public and personal compliance, these findings challenge the sustainability of strict regulatory measures. Moreover, young males were most likely to express low compliance, stressing the need for selective health promotion efforts. Finally, the positive association between public stigma and compliance points to potential othering effects of stigma during a pandemic, but further longitudinal research is required to examine its impact on health and social processes throughout the pandemic.
Over the last years, there has been a resurge in the interest to study the relationship between interoception and emotion. By now, it is well established that interoception contributes to the experience of emotions. However, it may also be possible that interoception contributes to the regulation of emotions. To test this possibility, we studied the relationship between interoception and emotion regulation in a sample of healthy individuals (n = 84). We used a similar heartbeat detection task and a similar self-report questionnaire for the assessment of interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation as in previous studies. In contrast to previous studies, we differentiated between male and female individuals in our analyses and controlled our analyses for individual characteristics that may affect the relationship between interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation. We found sex-differences in interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation that amounted to a sex-specific relationship between interoceptive accuracy and emotion regulation: Whereas interoceptive accuracy was related to reappraisal but not to suppression in male individuals, interoceptive accuracy was unrelated to reappraisal and suppression in female individuals. These findings indicate that the relationship between interoception and emotion regulation is far more complex than has been suggested by previous findings. However, these findings nonetheless support the view that interoception is essential for both, the regulation and experience of emotions.
Moral dilemmas often concern actions that involve causing harm to others in the attempt to prevent greater harm. But not all actions of this kind are equal in terms of their moral evaluation. In particular, a harm-causing preventive action is typically regarded as less acceptable if the harm is a means to achieve the goal of preventing greater harm than if it is a foreseen but unintended side-effect of the action. Likewise, a harm-causing preventive action is typically deemed less acceptable if it directly produces the harm than if it merely initiates a process that brings about the harmful consequence by its own dynamics. We report three experiments that investigated to which degree these two variables, the instrumentality of the harm (harm as means vs. side-effect; Experiments 1, 2, and 3) and personal force (personal vs. impersonal dilemmas; Experiments 2 and 3) influence deontological (harm-rejection) and utilitarian (outcome-maximization) inclinations that have been hypothesized to underly moral judgments in harm-related moral dilemmas. To measure these moral inclinations, the process dissociation procedure was used. The results suggest that the instrumentality of the harm and personal force affect both inclinations, but in opposite ways. Personal dilemmas and dilemmas characterized by harm as a means evoked higher deontological tendencies and lower utilitarian tendencies, than impersonal dilemmas and dilemmas where the harm was a side-effect. These distinct influences of the two dilemma conceptualization variables went undetected if the conventional measure of moral inclinations, the proportion of harm-accepting judgments, was analyzed. Furthermore, although deontological and utilitarian inclinations were found to be largely independent overall, there was some evidence that their correlation depended on the experimental conditions.
Much research has been devoted to the development of emotion recognition tests that can be used to investigate how individuals identify and discriminate emotional expressions of other individuals. One of the most prominent emotion recognition tests is the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RME-T). The original RME-T has been widely used to investigate how individuals recognize complex emotional expressions from the eye region of adult faces. However, the RME-T can only be used to investigate inter-individual differences in complex emotion recognition during the processing of adult faces. To extend its usefulness, we developed a modified version of the RME-T, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes of Children Test (RME-C-T). The RME-C-T can be used to investigate how individuals recognize complex emotional expressions from the eye region of child faces. However, the validity of the RME-C-T has not been evaluated yet. We, thus, administered the RME-C-T together with the RME-T to a sample of healthy adult participants (n = 119). The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) were also administered. Participants’ RME-C-T performance correlated with participants’ RME-T performance, implying that the RME-C-T measures similar emotion recognition abilities as the RME-T. Participants’ RME-C-T performance also correlated with participants’ IRI and TAS scores, indicating that these emotion recognition abilities are affected by empathetic and alexithymic traits. Moreover, participants’ RME-C-T performance differed between participants with high and low TAS scores, suggesting that the RME-C-T is sensitive enough to detect impairments in these emotion recognition abilities. The RME-C-T, thus, turned out to be a valid measure of inter-individual differences in complex emotion recognition during the processing of child faces.
Despite major research interest regarding gender differences in emotion regulation, it is still not clear whether men and women differ in their basic capacity to implement specific emotion regulation strategies, as opposed to indications of the habitual use of these strategies in self-reports. Similarly, little is known on how such basic capacities relate to indices of well-being in both sexes. This study took a novel approach by investigating gender differences in the capacity for generating cognitive reappraisals in adverse situations in a sample of 67 female and 59 male students, using a maximum performance test of the inventiveness in generating reappraisals. Participants’ self-perceived efficacy in emotion regulation was additionally assessed. Analyses showed that men and women did not differ in their basic capacity to generate alternative appraisals for anxiety-eliciting scenarios, suggesting similar functional cognitive mechanisms in the implementation of this strategy. Yet, higher cognitive reappraisal capacity predicted fewer depressive daily-life experiences in men only. These findings suggest that in the case of cognitive reappraisal, benefits for well-being in women might depend on a more complex combination of basic ability, habits, and efficacy-beliefs, along with the use of other emotion regulation strategies. The results of this study may have useful implications for psychotherapy research and practice.
In the present study, we investigated whether inter-individual differences in vagally mediated heart rate variability (vmHRV) would be associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. To this end, we determined resting state HF-HRV in 90 individuals that also completed questionnaires assessing inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. Our categorical and dimensional analyses revealed that inter-individual differences in HF-HRV were differently associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. We found that individuals with high HF-HRV reported more empathy and less alexithymia than individuals with low HF-HRV. Moreover, we even found that an increase in HF-HRV was associated with an increase in empathy and a decrease in alexithymia across all participants. Taken together, these findings indicate that individuals with high HF-HRV are more empathetic and less alexithymic than individuals with low HF-HRV. These differences in empathy and alexithymia may explain why individuals with high HF-HRV are more successful in sharing and understanding the mental and emotional states of others than individuals with low HF-HRV.
High-Frequency Binaural Beats Increase Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence from Dual-Task Crosstalk
(2016)
Increasing evidence suggests that cognitive-control processes can be configured to optimize either persistence of information processing (by amplifying competition between decision-making alternatives and top-down biasing of this competition) or flexibility (by dampening competition and biasing). We investigated whether high-frequency binaural beats, an auditory illusion suspected to act as a cognitive enhancer, have an impact on cognitive-control configuration. We hypothesized that binaural beats in the gamma range bias the cognitive-control style toward flexibility, which in turn should increase the crosstalk between tasks in a dual-task paradigm. We replicated earlier findings that the reaction time in the first-performed task is sensitive to the compatibility between the responses in the first and the second task—an indication of crosstalk. As predicted, exposing participants to binaural beats in the gamma range increased this effect as compared to a control condition in which participants were exposed to a continuous tone of 340 Hz. These findings provide converging evidence that the cognitive-control style can be systematically biased by inducing particular internal states; that high-frequency binaural beats bias the control style toward more flexibility; and that different styles are implemented by changing the strength of local competition and top-down bias.
The shared decline in cognitive abilities, sensory functions (e.g., vision and hearing), and physical health with increasing age is well documented with some research attributing this shared age-related decline to a single common cause (e.g., aging brain). We evaluate the extent to which the common cause hypothesis predicts associations between vision and physical health with social cognition abilities specifically face perception and face memory. Based on a sample of 443 adults (17–88 years old), we test a series of structural equation models, including Multiple Indicator Multiple Cause (MIMIC) models, and estimate the extent to which vision and self-reported physical health are related to face perception and face memory through a common factor, before and after controlling for their fluid cognitive component and the linear effects of age. Results suggest significant shared variance amongst these constructs, with a common factor explaining some, but not all, of the shared age-related variance. Also, we found that the relations of face perception, but not face memory, with vision and physical health could be completely explained by fluid cognition. Overall, results suggest that a single common cause explains most, but not all age-related shared variance with domain specific aging mechanisms evident.